86岁属什么生肖| 胃脘是什么意思| 烂尾是什么意思| ooc是什么意思| 哦吼是什么意思| 诚不我欺什么意思| 漪什么意思| 湿疹是什么原因引起的起的| 新疆是什么民族| 狗为什么喜欢吃屎| 王代表什么生肖| 老年斑用什么药膏可以去掉| 优思悦是什么药| 胡麻是什么植物| ls是什么牌子| 肾病综合症是什么病| 甲状腺是什么病严重吗| 打屁很臭是什么原因| 胸为什么会胀痛| 什么提示你怀了女宝宝| 牙疼吃什么药效果好| 公分是什么单位| 丹参有什么作用| 吃什么养胃| 四维彩超是检查什么| 什么样的月亮| 茉莉花有什么功效| ac代表什么意思| 区委常委是什么级别| 哭夫痣是什么意思| 珠胎暗结是什么意思| 邪是什么意思| 突然心慌是什么原因| 1987年出生属什么生肖| 11月24是什么星座| 女性乳房痒是什么原因| 松子吃了有什么好处和坏处| 乳腺癌长在什么位置| 吃了鸡蛋不能吃什么| 冠状动脉ct检查什么| 珍珠奶茶的珍珠是什么做的| 薄荷泡水喝有什么好处| 阴阳两虚是什么症状| 结婚的礼数都有什么| 梦见找鞋子是什么意思| 什么叫化疗为什么要化疗| 囊变是什么意思| 胎儿左心室强光点是什么意思| 狐假虎威告诉我们什么道理| lp 什么意思| 面霜和乳液有什么区别| 锦纹是什么中药| 为什么不| 本体是什么意思| 肺气不足吃什么食物可以补肺气| 什么人不适合艾灸| 湖南有什么特产| 紫五行属什么| 54年属什么| 无痛人流后吃什么对身体恢复比较好| 皮肤癣用什么药| 昏什么昏什么| 什么是三好学生| 什么鸟好养| 保家卫国是什么生肖| 高兴的动物是什么生肖| inr是什么意思| 下限是什么意思| 居住证是什么| 刀厄痣是什么意思| 四个月宝宝可以吃什么辅食| 美满霉素又叫什么名字| 1957年属什么生肖| 1950年属虎的是什么命| 印度讲什么语言| 肝火郁结是什么症状| 乳腺增生什么意思| 高血压高血糖挂什么科| 两肺纹理增重什么意思| 3月5日是什么纪念日| 为什么肚子会胀气| 蜜饯是什么东西| 朱砂红是什么颜色| 六月八日是什么星座| 元胡是什么| ra是什么意思| 人瘦肚子大是什么原因| 葛根粉有什么功效和作用| 殇什么意思| 周到是什么意思| nokia是什么牌子的手机| 或缺是什么意思| 皮肤出现红点是什么原因| 拍胸片能检查出什么| 簇新是什么意思| 胸闷气短是什么病| 摸不到心跳是什么情况| 文殊菩萨是管什么的| 爬行对身体有什么好处| 生肖猪和什么生肖相冲| 梦见火灾预示什么| 雷锋日是什么时候| 月经吃什么水果好| 什么情况下要打狂犬疫苗| 灯火葳蕤是什么意思| 泡脚对身体有什么好处| 脚有酸味是什么原因| 佛度有缘人是什么意思| 宝宝风热感冒吃什么药| 防晒霜什么牌子好| 波子是什么车| 小孩阑尾炎是由什么原因引起的| 内分泌失调挂什么科| 女人腰疼是什么原因引起的| 医院红色手环代表什么| 信佛有什么好处| 阙什么意思| 犬子是什么意思| 6月1号是什么星座| 肌肉劳损吃什么药| 痘坑用什么药膏最有效| 一颗颗什么| 小便尿道刺痛吃什么药| 怀孕做梦梦到蛇是什么意思| 东施效颦的意思是什么| 什么孩子该看心理医生| 乙型肝炎表面抗原阳性是什么意思| 双相情感障碍是什么病| 柏拉图之恋是什么意思| 打鼾是什么意思| 脑溢血是什么原因引起的| 腊肉炖什么好吃| 摘胆对身体有什么影响| 宋五行属什么| 牙齿松动吃什么药| 口水多是什么原因引起的| 什么是低筋面粉| 尿酸高什么水果不能吃| 产奶速度慢是什么原因| 口臭是什么原因导致的呢| 滤泡性咽炎吃什么药| 什么是气| 什么是自由度| 蛇五行属什么| 虎和什么属相不合| 什么叫台风| 初级会计什么时候拿证| tpp是什么意思| 布洛芬有什么副作用| 原发性是什么意思| 四川酸菜是什么菜| rl是什么意思| hdl是什么意思| 吃了避孕药会有什么副作用| 女性口臭都是什么原因| 罡什么意思| 牙冠是什么意思| 大米饭配什么菜最好吃| 什么是pp材质| 生理期没来是什么原因| 富丽堂皇是什么意思| 海参什么时间吃最好| 皮肤湿疹用什么药膏| 视网膜为什么会脱落| 水中毒是什么症状| 头顶长白头发是什么原因造成的| 胃溃疡吃什么中成药| 镁低了是什么原因| 球菌是什么意思| 梦见打仗是什么意思| moi是什么意思| 唇干是什么原因引起的| 老出虚汗是什么原因| 心肌梗塞是什么原因引起的| 联通查流量发什么短信| 耳朵嗡嗡的响是什么原因| lv中文名叫什么| 早上起来嘴巴发苦是什么原因| 膜性肾病什么意思| 移植后需要注意什么| 兔配什么生肖最好| 萎谢是什么意思| 河马吃什么| 失眠多梦用什么药| 60年是什么婚| 什么牙什么嘴| 金蟾是什么| 茉莉花什么时候开花| 睾丸肿大吃什么药| 手指指尖发麻是什么原因| 咖喱块什么牌子的好| 微博是什么意思| puppies什么意思| 雷锋代表什么生肖| 打喷嚏是什么意思| wb是什么| 大便出血是什么原因引起的| 饷是什么意思| 六月初一什么日子| 淘宝预售是什么意思| 什么时候是排卵期| 战区司令员是什么级别| 三个王念什么| 百合与什么搭配最好| 梅核气吃什么药好得快| 立冬是什么时候| rna检测是什么意思| 外阴白斑用什么药最好| 91岁属什么| 蜈蚣长什么样子| 十指连心是什么意思| 女命正印代表什么| 6d是什么意思| 经期洗澡有什么影响| 释怀什么意思| 心肝火旺吃什么中成药| slogan什么意思| 上腹疼是什么原因| 女生的隐私长什么样子| 岁月如歌是什么意思| 脾肾阳虚吃什么中成药最好| 焱加木念什么| 老感冒是什么原因| member是什么意思| 英语一和英语二有什么区别| 口腔溃疡反复发作是什么原因| 吃什么能养肝护肝| 什么样的人容易猝死| 小便有血是什么原因| 磁共振是做什么的| 查输卵管通不通做什么检查| 胎教什么时候开始最好| 氯中毒吃什么可以解毒| 芈月是秦始皇什么人| 味增是什么| 酚氨咖敏片的别名叫什么| 手上掉皮什么原因| 更年期什么时候| 古灵精怪什么意思| 狗发烧吃什么药| 优生优育是什么意思| 吕布是什么生肖| 风湿类风湿有什么症状表现| 脸上长痣是什么原因| 食管憩室是什么病| 咳嗽一直不好什么原因| 马和驴为什么能杂交| 半什么半什么的成语| 眼睛肿痛什么原因| 为什么丰胸霜一抹就变大| 骞字五行属什么| 罹患率是什么意思| 爆竹声中一岁除下一句是什么| 露酒是什么意思| 蜂蜜和什么食物相克| 女人手心热吃什么药好| 女人长期喝西洋参有什么好处| 聪明反被聪明误是什么意思| 假小子是什么意思| 射手属于什么象星座| 为什么会肠鸣| 贪是什么意思| 三季人是什么意思| 腰间盘突出挂什么科室| 百度

《婚姻法》新解释夫妻共债共签 专家:保护家庭成员个体权益

(Redirected from Inflect)
百度   保时捷销售与市场执行委员会成员DetlevvonPlaten说:JensPuttfarcken曾经从事过不同的管理类工作,拥有相当丰富的销售经验。

In linguistic morphology, inflection (less commonly, inflexion) is a process of word formation[1] in which a word is modified to express different grammatical categories such as tense, case, voice, aspect, person, number, gender, mood, animacy, and definiteness.[2] The inflection of verbs is called conjugation, while the inflection of nouns, adjectives, adverbs, etc.[a] can be called declension.

Inflection of the Scottish Gaelic lexeme for 'dog', which is for singular, chù for dual with the number ('two'), and coin for plural

An inflection expresses grammatical categories with affixation (such as prefix, suffix, infix, circumfix, and transfix), apophony (as Indo-European ablaut), or other modifications.[3] For example, the Latin verb ducam, meaning "I will lead", includes the suffix -am, expressing person (first), number (singular), and tense-mood (future indicative or present subjunctive). The use of this suffix is an inflection. In contrast, in the English clause "I will lead", the word lead is not inflected for any of person, number, or tense; it is simply the bare form of a verb. The inflected form of a word often contains both one or more free morphemes (a unit of meaning which can stand by itself as a word), and one or more bound morphemes (a unit of meaning which cannot stand alone as a word). For example, the English word cars is a noun that is inflected for number, specifically to express the plural; the content morpheme car is unbound because it could stand alone as a word, while the suffix -s is bound because it cannot stand alone as a word. These two morphemes together form the inflected word cars.

Words that are never subject to inflection are said to be invariant; for example, the English verb must is an invariant item: it never takes a suffix or changes form to signify a different grammatical category. Its categories can be determined only from its context. Languages that seldom make use of inflection, such as English, are said to be analytic. Analytic languages that do not make use of derivational morphemes, such as Standard Chinese, are said to be isolating.

Requiring the forms or inflections of more than one word in a sentence to be compatible with each other according to the rules of the language is known as concord or agreement. For example, in "the man jumps", "man" is a singular noun, so "jump" is constrained in the present tense to use the third person singular suffix "s".

Languages that have some degree of inflection are synthetic languages. They can be highly inflected (such as Georgian or Kichwa), moderately inflected (such as Russian or Latin), weakly inflected (such as English), but not uninflected (such as Chinese). Languages that are so inflected that a sentence can consist of a single highly inflected word (such as many Native American languages) are called polysynthetic languages. Languages in which each inflection conveys only a single grammatical category, such as Finnish, are known as agglutinative languages, while languages in which a single inflection can convey multiple grammatical roles (such as both nominative case and plural, as in Latin and German) are called fusional.

Examples in English

edit

In English most nouns are inflected for number with the inflectional plural affix -s (as in "dog" → "dog-s"), and most English verbs are inflected for tense with the inflectional past tense affix -ed (as in "call" → "call-ed"). English also inflects verbs by affixation to mark the third person singular in the present tense (with -s), and the present participle (with -ing). English short adjectives are inflected to mark comparative and superlative forms (with -er and -est respectively).

There are eight regular inflectional affixes in the English language.[4][5]

Inflectional affixes in English
Affix Grammatical category Mark Part of speech
-s Number plural nouns
-'s/'/s Case genitive nouns and noun phrases, pronouns (marks independent genitive)
-ing Aspect progressive gerunds or participles
-en/-ed Aspect perfect verbs
-ed/-t Tense past (simple) verbs
-s Person, number, aspect, tense 3rd person singular present indicative verbs
-er Degree of comparison comparative adjectives and adverbs
-est Degree of comparison superlative adjectives and adverbs

Despite the march toward regularization, modern English retains traces of its ancestry, with a minority of its words still using inflection by ablaut (sound change, mostly in verbs) and umlaut (a particular type of sound change, mostly in nouns), as well as long-short vowel alternation. For example:

  • Write, wrote, written (marking by ablaut variation, and also suffixing in the participle)
  • Sing, sang, sung (ablaut)
  • Foot, feet (marking by umlaut variation)
  • Mouse, mice (umlaut)
  • Child, children (ablaut, and also suffixing in the plural)

For details, see English plural, English verbs, and English irregular verbs.

Regular and irregular inflection

edit

When a given word class is subject to inflection in a particular language, there are generally one or more standard patterns of inflection (the paradigms described below) that words in that class may follow. Words which follow such a standard pattern are said to be regular; those that inflect differently are called irregular.

For instance, many languages that feature verb inflection have both regular verbs and irregular verbs. In English, regular verbs form their past tense and past participle with the ending -[e]d. Therefore, verbs like play, arrive and enter are regular, while verbs like sing, keep and go are irregular. Irregular verbs often preserve patterns that were regular in past forms of the language, but which have now become anomalous; in rare cases, there are regular verbs that were irregular in past forms of the language. (For more details see English verbs and English irregular verbs.)

Other types of irregular inflected form include irregular plural nouns, such as the English mice, children and women (see English plural) and the French yeux (the plural of ?il, "eye"); and irregular comparative and superlative forms of adjectives or adverbs, such as the English better and best (which correspond to the positive form good or well).

Irregularities can have four basic causes:[citation needed]

  1. euphony: Regular inflection would result in forms that sound esthetically unpleasing or are difficult to pronounce (English farfarther or further, Spanish tenertengo, tendré vs. comercomo, comeré, Portuguese vs. Spanish andar → Portuguese andaram vs. Spanish anduvieron).
  2. principal parts: These are generally considered to have been formed independently of one another, so the student must memorize them when learning a new word. Example: Latin dīcō, dīcere, dīxī, dictum → Spanish digo, decir, dije, dicho.
  3. strong vs. weak inflection: In some cases, two inflection systems exist, conventionally classified as "strong" and "weak." For instance, English and German have weak verbs that form the past tense and past participle by adding an ending (English jumpjumped, German machenmachte) and strong verbs that change vowel, and in some cases form the past participle by adding -en (English swimswam, swum, German schwimmenschwamm, geschwommen). Ancient Greek verbs are likewise said to have had a first aorist (?λ?σα) and a second aorist (?λιπον).
  4. suppletion: The "irregular" form was originally derived from a different root (English personpeople). The comparative and superlative forms of good in many languages display this phenomenon (e.g. eng. good, better, best).

For more details on some of the considerations that apply to regularly and irregularly inflected forms, see the article on regular and irregular verbs.

Declension and conjugation

edit

Two traditional grammatical terms refer to inflections of specific word classes:

An organized list of the inflected forms of a given lexeme or root word is called its declension if it is a noun, or its conjugation if it is a verb.

Below is the declension of the English pronoun I, which is inflected for case and number.

singular plural
nominative I we
oblique me us
possessive determiner my our
possessive pronoun mine ours
reflexive myself ourselves

The pronoun who is also inflected according to case. Its declension is defective, in the sense that it lacks a reflexive form.

singular and plural
nominative who
oblique whom (traditional), who (informal)
possessive whose
reflexive

The following table shows the conjugation of the verb to arrive in the indicative mood: suffixes inflect it for person, number, and tense:

Tense I you he, she, it we you they
Present arrive arrive arrives arrive arrive arrive
Past arrived arrived arrived arrived arrived arrived

The non-finite forms arrive (bare infinitive), arrived (past participle) and arriving (gerund/present participle), although not inflected for person or number, can also be regarded as part of the conjugation of the verb to arrive. Compound verb forms, such as I have arrived, I had arrived, or I will arrive, can be included also in the conjugation of the verb for didactic purposes, but they are not overt inflections of arrive. The formula for deriving the covert form, in which the relevant inflections do not occur in the main verb, is

pronoun + conjugated auxiliary verb + non-finite form of main verb.

Inflectional paradigm

edit

An inflectional paradigm refers to a pattern (usually a set of inflectional endings), where a class of words follow the same pattern. Nominal inflectional paradigms are called declensions, and verbal inflectional paradigms are termed conjugations. For instance, there are five types of Latin declension. Words that belong to the first declension usually end in -a and are usually feminine. These words share a common inflectional framework. In Old English, nouns are divided into two major categories of declension, the strong and weak ones, as shown below:

gender and number
Masculine Neuter Feminine
Singular Plural Singular Plural Singular Plural
case Strong noun declension
engel 'angel' scip 'ship' sorg 'sorrow'
Nominative engel englas scip scipu sorg sorga
Accusative engel englas scip scipu sorge sorga/sorge
Genitive engles engla scipes scipa sorge sorga
Dative engle englum scipe scipum sorge sorgum
case Weak noun declension
nama 'name' ēage 'eye' tunge 'tongue'
Nominative nama naman ēage ēagan tunge tungan
Accusative naman naman ēage ēagan tungan tungan
Genitive naman namena ēagan ēagena tungan tungena
Dative naman namum ēagan ēagum tungan tungum

The terms "strong declension" and "weak declension" are primarily relevant to well-known dependent-marking languages[citation needed] (such as the Indo-European languages,[citation needed] or Japanese). In dependent-marking languages, nouns in adpositional (prepositional or postpositional) phrases can carry inflectional morphemes.

In head-marking languages, the adpositions can carry the inflection in adpositional phrases. This means that these languages will have inflected adpositions. In Western Apache (San Carlos dialect), the postposition -ká’ 'on' is inflected for person and number with prefixes:

Singular Dual Plural
1st shi- on me noh- on us two da-noh- 'on us'
2nd ni- on you nohwi- 'on you two' da-nohwi- 'on you all'
3rd bi- 'on him' da-bi- 'on them'

Traditional grammars have specific terms for inflections of nouns and verbs but not for those of adpositions.[clarification needed]

Compared to derivation

edit

Inflection is the process of adding inflectional morphemes that modify a verb's tense, mood, aspect, voice, person, or number or a noun's case, gender, or number, rarely affecting the word's meaning or class. Examples of applying inflectional morphemes to words are adding -s to the root dog to form dogs and adding -ed to wait to form waited.

In contrast, derivation is the process of adding derivational morphemes, which create a new word from existing words and change the semantic meaning or the part of speech of the affected word, such as by changing a noun to a verb.[6]

Distinctions between verbal moods are mainly indicated by derivational morphemes.

Words are rarely listed in dictionaries on the basis of their inflectional morphemes (in which case they would be lexical items). However, they often are listed on the basis of their derivational morphemes. For instance, English dictionaries list readable and readability, words with derivational suffixes, along with their root read. However, no traditional English dictionary lists book as one entry and books as a separate entry; the same goes for jump and jumped.

Inflectional morphology

edit

Languages that add inflectional morphemes to words are sometimes called inflectional languages, which is a synonym for inflected languages. Morphemes may be added in several different ways:

Inflection through reduplication

edit

Reduplication is a morphological process where a constituent is repeated. The direct repetition of a word or root is called total reduplication (or full reduplication). The repetition of a segment is referred to as partial reduplication. Reduplication can serve both derivational and inflectional functions. A few examples are given below:

Inflectional Reduplication
Value Language Original Reduplicated
Plurality Indonesian[7] buku 'book' buku-buku 'books'
Distribution Standard Chinese[8] ren24 'person' ren24 ren24 'everyone'
Intensity Taiwanese Hokkien[9] ang24 'red' ang24 ang24 'reddish'
Imperfective Ilokano[10] ag-bása 'read' ag-basbása 'reading'
Inchoative Nukuoro[10] gohu 'dark' gohu-gohu 'getting dark'
Progressive Pazeh language[11] bazu’ 'wash' baabazu’ 'be washing'

Inflection through tone change

edit

Palancar and Léonard provided an example with Tlatepuzco Chinantec (an Oto-Manguean language spoken in Southern Mexico), where tones are able to distinguish mood, person, and number:[12][13]

Verb paradigm of 'bend' in Tlatepuzco Chinantec
1 SG 1 PL 2 3
Completive hú?1 hú?13 hú?1 hú?2
Incompletive hú?12 hú?12 hú?12 hú?2
Irrealis hú?13 hú?13 hú?13 hú?2

Case can be distinguished with tone as well, as in Maasai language (a Nilo-Saharan language spoken in Kenya and Tanzania) (Hyman, 2016):[14]

Case Inflection in Maasai
gloss Nominative Accusative
'head' èl??k??nyá èl??k??nyá
'rat' èndérònì èndèrónì

In various languages

edit

Indo-European languages (fusional)

edit

Because the Proto-Indo-European language was highly inflected, all of its descendant Indo-European languages, such as Albanian, Armenian, English, German, Ukrainian, Russian, Persian, Kurdish, Italian, Irish, Spanish, French, Hindi, Marathi, Urdu, Bengali, and Nepali, are inflected to a greater or lesser extent. In general, older Indo-European languages such as Latin, Ancient Greek, Old English, Old Norse, Old Church Slavonic and Sanskrit are extensively inflected because of their temporal proximity to Proto-Indo-European. Deflexion has caused modern versions of some Indo-European languages that were previously highly inflected to be much less so; an example is Modern English, as compared to Old English. In general, languages where deflexion occurs replace inflectional complexity with more rigorous word order, which provides the lost inflectional details. Most Slavic languages and some Indo-Aryan languages are an exception to the general Indo-European deflexion trend, continuing to be highly inflected (in some cases acquiring additional inflectional complexity and grammatical genders, as in Czech & Marathi).

English

edit

Old English was a moderately inflected language, using an extensive case system similar to that of modern Icelandic, Faroese or German. Middle and Modern English lost progressively more of the Old English inflectional system. Modern English is considered a weakly inflected language, since its nouns have only vestiges of inflection (plurals, the pronouns), and its regular verbs have only four forms: an inflected form for the past indicative and subjunctive (looked), an inflected form for the third-person-singular present indicative (looks), an inflected form for the present participle (looking), and an uninflected form for everything else (look). While the English possessive indicator 's (as in "Jennifer's book") is a remnant of the Old English genitive case suffix, it is now considered by syntacticians not to be a suffix but a clitic,[15] although some linguists argue that it has properties of both.[16]

Scandinavian languages

edit

Old Norse was inflected, but modern Swedish, Norwegian, and Danish have lost much of their inflection. Grammatical case has largely died out with the exception of pronouns, just like English. However, adjectives, nouns, determiners and articles still have different forms according to grammatical number and grammatical gender. Danish and Swedish only inflect for two different genders while Norwegian has to some degree retained the feminine forms and inflects for three grammatical genders like Icelandic. However, in comparison to Icelandic, there are considerably fewer feminine forms left in the language.

In comparison, Icelandic preserves almost all of the inflections of Old Norse and remains heavily inflected. It retains all the grammatical cases from Old Norse and is inflected for number and three different grammatical genders. The dual number forms are however almost completely lost in comparison to Old Norse.

Unlike other Germanic languages, nouns are inflected for definiteness in all Scandinavian languages, like in the following case for Norwegian (nynorsk):

Inflection of nouns in Norwegian (nynorsk)
Singular Plural
Indefinite Definite Indefinite Definite
masculine ein bil bilen bilar bilane
a car the car cars the cars
feminine ei vogn vogna vogner vognene
a wagon the wagon wagons the wagons
neuter eit hus huset hus husa
a house the house houses the houses
Articles in Norwegian (nynorsk)
Singular Plural
Indefinite Definite Indefinite Definite
masculine ein -en -ar -ane
feminine ei -a -er -ene
neuter eit -et - -a

Adjectives and participles are also inflected for definiteness in all Scandinavian languages like in Proto-Germanic.

Other Germanic languages

edit

Modern German remains moderately inflected, retaining four noun cases, although the genitive started falling into disuse in all but formal writing in Early New High German. The case system of Dutch, simpler than that of German, is also simplified in common usage. Afrikaans, recognized as a distinct language in its own right rather than a Dutch dialect only in the early 20th century, has lost almost all inflection.

Latin and the Romance languages

edit

The Romance languages, such as Spanish, Italian, French, Portuguese and especially – with its many cases – Romanian, have more overt inflection than English, especially in verb conjugation. Adjectives, nouns and articles are considerably less inflected than verbs, but they still have different forms according to number and grammatical gender.

Latin, the mother tongue of the Romance languages, was highly inflected; nouns and adjectives had different forms according to seven grammatical cases (including five major ones) with five major patterns of declension, and three genders instead of the two found in most Romance tongues. There were four patterns of conjugation in six tenses, three moods (indicative, subjunctive, imperative, plus the infinitive, participle, gerund, gerundive, and supine) and two voices (passive and active), all overtly expressed by affixes (passive voice forms were periphrastic in three tenses).

Baltic languages

edit

The Baltic languages are highly inflected. Nouns and adjectives are declined in up to seven overt cases. Additional cases are defined in various covert ways. For example, an inessive case, an illative case, an adessive case and allative case are borrowed from Finnic. Latvian has only one overt locative case but it syncretizes the above four cases to the locative marking them by differences in the use of prepositions.[17] Lithuanian breaks them out of the genitive case, accusative case and locative case by using different postpositions.[18]

Dual form is obsolete in standard Latvian and nowadays it is also considered nearly obsolete in standard Lithuanian. For instance, in standard Lithuanian it is normal to say "dvi varnos (plural) – two crows" instead of "dvi varni (dual)". Adjectives, pronouns, and numerals are declined for number, gender, and case to agree with the noun they modify or for which they substitute. Baltic verbs are inflected for tense, mood, aspect, and voice. They agree with the subject in person and number (not in all forms in modern Latvian).

Slavic languages

edit

All Slavic languages make use of a high degree of inflection, typically having six or seven cases and three genders for nouns and adjectives. However, the overt case system has disappeared almost completely in modern Bulgarian and Macedonian. Most verb tenses and moods are also formed by inflection (however, some are periphrastic, typically the future and conditional). Inflection is also present in adjective comparation and word derivation.

Declensional endings depend on case (nominative, genitive, dative, accusative, locative, instrumental, vocative), number (singular, dual or plural), gender (masculine, feminine, neuter) and animacy (animate vs inanimate). Unusual in other language families, declension in most Slavic languages also depends on whether the word is a noun or an adjective. Slovene and Sorbian languages use a rare third number, (in addition to singular and plural numbers) known as dual (in case of some words dual survived also in Polish and other Slavic languages). Modern Russian, Serbian and Czech also use a more complex form of dual, but this misnomer applies instead to numbers 2, 3, 4, and larger numbers ending in 2, 3, or 4 (with the exception of the teens, which are handled as plural; thus, 102 is dual, but 12 or 127 are not). In addition, in some Slavic languages, such as Polish, word stems are frequently modified by the addition or absence of endings, resulting in consonant and vowel alternation.

Arabic (fusional)

edit

Modern Standard Arabic (also called Literary Arabic) is an inflected language. It uses a system of independent and suffix pronouns classified by person and number and verbal inflections marking person and number. Suffix pronouns are used as markers of possession and as objects of verbs and prepositions. The tatweel (???) marks where the verb stem, verb form, noun, or preposition is placed.[19]

Singular Plural Dual
Independent
Pronoun
Suffix
Pronoun
Present Tense
Affix
Independent
Pronoun
Suffix
Pronoun
Present Tense
Affix
Independent
Pronoun
Suffix
Pronoun
Present Tense
Affix
Person First ????? ?anā "I" ??????, ?????, ??????
—ī, —ya, —nī
? ?— ?????? na?nu ?????? —nā ???? n— same as plural
Second masc. ?????? ?anta "you" ????? —ka ???? t— ???????? ?antum ??????? —kum ????????? t—ūn ????????? ?antumā ???????? —kumā ????????? t—āni
fem. ?????? ?anti "you" ????? —ki ????????? t—īna ????????? ?antunna ???????? —kunna ???????? t—na
Third masc. ???? huwa "he" ????? —hu ???? y— ???? hum ??????? —hum ????????? y—ūna ????? humā ???????? —humā ????????? y—āni
fem. ???? hiya "she" ?????? —hā ???? t— ????? hunna ???????? —hunna ???????? t—na

Arabic regional dialects (e.g. Moroccan Arabic, Egyptian Arabic, Gulf Arabic), used for everyday communication, tend to have less inflection than the more formal Literary Arabic. For example, in Jordanian Arabic, the second- and third-person feminine plurals (????? antunna and ??? hunna) and their respective unique conjugations are lost and replaced by the masculine (???? antum and ?? hum), whereas in Lebanese and Syrian Arabic, ?? hum is replaced by ??? hunna.

In addition, the system known as ?I?rāb places vowel suffixes on each verb, noun, adjective, and adverb, according to its function within a sentence and its relation to surrounding words.[19]

Uralic languages (agglutinative)

edit

The Uralic languages are agglutinative, following from the agglutination in Proto-Uralic. The largest languages are Hungarian, Finnish, and Estonian—all European Union official languages. Uralic inflection is, or is developed from, affixing. Grammatical markers directly added to the word perform the same function as prepositions in English. Almost all words are inflected according to their roles in the sentence: verbs, nouns, pronouns, numerals, adjectives, and some particles.

Hungarian and Finnish, in particular, often simply concatenate suffixes. For example, Finnish talossanikinko "in my house, too?" consists of talo-ssa-ni-kin-ko. However, in the Finnic languages (Finnish, Estonian etc.) and the Sami languages, there are processes which affect the root, particularly consonant gradation. The original suffixes may disappear (and appear only by liaison), leaving behind the modification of the root. This process is extensively developed in Estonian and Sami, and makes them also inflected, not only agglutinating languages. The Estonian illative case, for example, is expressed by a modified root: majamajja (historical form *maja-han).

Altaic languages (agglutinative)

edit

Though Altaic is widely considered to be a sprachbund by linguists, three language families united by a small subset of linguists as the Altaic language familyTurkic, Mongolic, and Manchu-Tungus—are agglutinative. The largest languages are Turkish, Azerbaijani and Uzbek—all Turkic languages. Altaic inflection is, or is developed from, affixing. Grammatical markers directly added to the word perform the same function as prepositions in English. Almost all words are inflected according to their roles in the sentence: verbs, nouns, pronouns, numerals, adjectives, and some particles.

Basque (agglutinative nominal inflection / fusional verb inflection)

edit

Basque, a language isolate, is a highly inflected language, heavily inflecting both nouns and verbs.

Noun phrase morphology is agglutinative and consists of suffixes which simply attach to the end of a stem (and more generally, only once at the very end of the nominal syntagma). These suffixes are in many cases fused with the article (-a for singular and -ak for plural), which in general is required to close a noun phrase in Basque if no other determiner is present, and unlike an article in many languages, it can only partially be correlated with the concept of definiteness. Proper nouns do not take an article, and indefinite nouns without the article (called mugagabe in Basque grammar) are highly restricted syntactically. Basque is an ergative language, meaning that inflectionally the single argument (subject) of an intransitive verb is marked in the same way as the direct object of a transitive verb. This is called the absolutive case and in Basque, as in most ergative languages, it is realized with a zero morph; in other words, it receives no special inflection. The subject of a transitive verb receives a special case suffix, called the ergative case.[20]

There is no case marking concord in Basque and case suffixes, including those fused with the article, are added only to the last word in a noun phrase. Plurality is not marked on the noun and is identified only in the article or other determiner, possibly fused with a case marker. The examples below are in the absolutive case with zero case marking, and include the article only:[20]

txakurr-a (the/a) dog
txakurr-ak (the) dogs
txakur polit-a (the/a) pretty dog
txakur polit-ak (the) pretty dogs

The noun phrase is declined for 11 cases: Absolutive, ergative, dative, possessive-genitive, benefactive, comitative, instrumental, inessive, allative, ablative, and local-genitive. These are signaled by suffixes that vary according to the categories of Singular, Plural, Indefinite, and Proper Noun, and many vary depending on whether the stem ends in a consonant or vowel. The Singular and Plural categories are fused with the article, and these endings are used when the noun phrase is not closed by any other determiner. This gives a potential 88 different forms, but the Indefinite and Proper Noun categories are identical in all but the local cases (inessive, allative, ablative, local-genitive), and many other variations in the endings can be accounted for by phonological rules operating to avoid impermissible consonant clusters. Local case endings are not normally added to animate Proper Nouns. The precise meaning of the local cases can be further specified by additional suffixes added after the local case suffixes.[20]

Verb forms are extremely complex, agreeing with the subject, direct object, and indirect object; and include forms that agree with a "dative of interest" for intransitive verbs as well as allocutive forms where the verb form is altered if one is speaking to a close acquaintance. These allocutive forms also have different forms depending on whether the addressee is male or female. This is the only area in Basque grammar where gender plays any role at all.[20] Subordination could also plausibly be considered an inflectional category of the Basque verb since subordination is signaled by prefixes and suffixes on the conjugated verb, further multiplying the number of potential forms.[21]

Transitivity is a thoroughgoing division of Basque verbs, and it is necessary to know the transitivity of a particular verb in order to conjugate it successfully. In the spoken language only a handful of commonly used verbs are fully conjugated in the present and simple past, most verbs being conjugated by means of an auxiliary which differs according to transitivity. The literary language includes a few more such verbs, but the number is still very small. Even these few verbs require an auxiliary to conjugate other tenses besides the present and simple past.[20]

The most common intransitive auxiliary is izan, which is also the verb for "to be". The most common transitive auxiliary is ukan, which is also the verb for "to have". (Other auxiliaries can be used in some of the tenses and may vary by dialect.) The compound tenses use an invariable form of the main verb (which appears in different forms according to the "tense group") and a conjugated form of the auxiliary. Pronouns are normally omitted if recoverable from the verb form. A couple of examples will have to suffice to demonstrate the complexity of the Basque verb:[20]

Liburu-ak

Book-PL.the

saldu

sell

dizkiegu.

AUX.3PL/ABS.3PL/DAT.1PL/ERG

Liburu-ak saldu dizkiegu.

Book-PL.the sell AUX.3PL/ABS.3PL/DAT.1PL/ERG

"We sold the books to them."

Kafe-a

Coffee-the

gusta-tzen

please-HAB

zaidak.

AUX.ALLOC/M.3SG/ABS.1SG/DAT

Kafe-a gusta-tzen zaidak.

Coffee-the please-HAB AUX.ALLOC/M.3SG/ABS.1SG/DAT

"I like coffee." ("Coffee pleases me.") (Used when speaking to a male friend.)

The morphs that represent the various tense/person/case/mood categories of Basque verbs, especially in the auxiliaries, are so highly fused that segmenting them into individual meaningful units is nearly impossible, if not pointless. Considering the multitude of forms that a particular Basque verb can take, it seems unlikely that an individual speaker would have an opportunity to utter them all in his or her lifetime.[22]

Mainland Southeast Asian languages (isolating)

edit

Most languages in the Mainland Southeast Asia linguistic area (such as the varieties of Chinese, Vietnamese, and Thai) are not overtly inflected, or show very little overt inflection, and are therefore considered analytic languages (also known as isolating languages).

Chinese

edit

Standard Chinese does not possess overt inflectional morphology. While some languages indicate grammatical relations with inflectional morphemes, Chinese utilizes word order and particles. Consider the following examples:

  • Latin:
    • Puer puellam videt.
    • Puellam puer videt.

Both sentences mean 'The boy sees the girl.' This is because puer (boy) is singular nominative, puellam (girl) is singular accusative. Since the roles of puer and puellam have been marked with case endings, the change in position does not matter.

  • Modern Standard Chinese:
    • 我给了他一本书 (wǒ gěile tā yī běn shū) 'I gave him a book'
    • 他给了我一本书 (tā gěile wǒ yī běn shū) 'He gave me a book'

The situation is very different in Chinese. Since Modern Chinese makes no use of inflection, the meanings of ('I' or 'me') and ('he' or 'him') shall be determined with their position.

In Classical Chinese, pronouns were overtly inflected to mark case. However, these overt case forms are no longer used; most of the alternative pronouns are considered archaic in modern Mandarin Chinese. Classically, 我 () was used solely as the first person accusative. 吾 () was generally used as the first person nominative.[23]

Certain varieties of Chinese are known to express meaning by means of tone change, although further investigations are required[dubiousdiscuss]. Note that the tone change must be distinguished from tone sandhi. Tone sandhi is a compulsory change that occurs when certain tones are juxtaposed. Tone change, however, is a morphologically conditioned alternation and is used as an inflectional or a derivational strategy. Examples from Taishan and Zhongshan (both Yue dialects spoken in Guangdong Province) are shown below:[24]

  • Taishan
ngwoi33 ‘I’ (singular)
ngwoi22 ‘we’ (plural)
  • Zhongshan
hy22 ‘go’
hy35 ‘gone’ (perfective)

The following table compares the personal pronouns of Sixian dialect (a dialect of Taiwanese Hakka)[25] with Zaiwa and Jingpho[26] (both Tibeto-Burman languages spoken in Yunnan and Burma). The superscripted numbers indicate the Chao tone numerals.

Comparison of Personal Pronouns
Sixian Zaiwa Jingpho
1 Nom ?ai11 ?o51 ?ai33
1 Gen ?a24 or ?ai11 ke55 ?a55 ?je?55
1 Acc ?ai11 ?o31 ?ai33
2 Nom ??11 na?51 na?33
2 Gen ?ia24 or ??11 ke55 na?55 na?55
2 Acc ??11 na?31 na?33
3 Nom ki11 ja?31 khji33
3 Gen kia24 or ki11 ke55 ja?51 khji?55
3 Acc ki11 ja?31 khji33

In Shanghainese, the third-person singular pronoun is overtly inflected as to case and the first- and second-person singular pronouns exhibit a change in tone depending on case.[citation needed]

Japanese (agglutinative)

edit

Japanese shows a high degree of overt inflection of verbs, less so of adjectives, and very little of nouns, but it is mostly strictly agglutinative and extremely regular. Fusion of morphemes also happen in colloquial speech, for example: the causative-passive ?せられ? (-serare-) fuses into ?され? (-sare-), as in 行かされ (ikasareru, "is made to go"), and the non-past progressive ?ている (-teiru) fuses into ?てる (-teru) as in 食べてる (tabeteru, "is eating"). Formally, every noun phrase must be marked for case, but this is done by invariable particles (clitic postpositions). (Many[citation needed] grammarians consider Japanese particles to be separate words, and therefore not an inflection, while others[citation needed] consider agglutination a type of overt inflection, and therefore consider Japanese nouns as overtly inflected.)

Auxiliary languages

edit

Some auxiliary languages, such as Lingua Franca Nova, Glosa, and Frater, have no inflection. Other auxiliary languages, such as Esperanto, Ido, and Interlingua have comparatively simple inflectional systems. Some, like Volapük, are on the contrary highly inflected (though perfectly regular).

Esperanto

edit

In Esperanto, an agglutinative language, nouns and adjectives are inflected for case (nominative, accusative) and number (singular, plural), according to a simple paradigm without irregularities. Verbs are not inflected for person or number, but they are inflected for tense (past, present, future) and mood (indicative, infinitive, conditional, jussive). They also form active and passive participles, which may be past, present or future. All verbs are regular.

Ido has a different form for each verbal tense (past, present, future, volitive and imperative) plus an infinitive, and both a present and past participle. There are though no verbal inflections for person or number, and all verbs are regular.

Nouns are marked for number (singular and plural), and the accusative case may be shown in certain situations, typically when the direct object of a sentence precedes its verb. On the other hand, adjectives are unmarked for gender, number or case (unless they stand on their own, without a noun, in which case they take on the same desinences as the missing noun would have taken). The definite article "la" ("the") remains unaltered regardless of gender or case, and also of number, except when there is no other word to show plurality. Pronouns are identical in all cases, though exceptionally the accusative case may be marked, as for nouns.

Interlingua

edit

Interlingua, in contrast with the Romance languages, has almost no irregular verb conjugations, and its verb forms are the same for all persons and numbers. It does, however, have compound verb tenses similar to those in the Romance, Germanic, and Slavic languages: ille ha vivite, "he has lived"; illa habeva vivite, "she had lived". Nouns are inflected by number, taking a plural -s, but rarely by gender: only when referring to a male or female being. Interlingua has no noun-adjective agreement by gender, number, or case. As a result, adjectives ordinarily have no inflections. They may take the plural form if they are being used in place of a noun: le povres, "the poor".

See also

edit

Notes

edit

Citations

edit

Footnotes

edit
  1. ^ Crystal, David. (2008). A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics (6th ed., pp. 243–244). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
  2. ^ Owens, Jonathan (1998). "Case and proto-Arabic, Part I". Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African Studies. 61: 51–73. doi:10.1017/S0041977X00015755. S2CID 204970487.
  3. ^ Brinton, Laurel J. (2000). The Structure of Modern English: A Linguistic Introduction. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. p. 104. ISBN 9781556196621.
  4. ^ "Section 4: Inflectional Morphemes". Analyzing Grammar in Context. University of Nevada, Las Vegas. Retrieved 11 March 2024.
  5. ^ "Derivation and Inflection" (PDF). Retrieved 11 March 2024 – via websites.umich.edu/~jlawler.
  6. ^ Anderson, Stephen R. (1985), "Inflectional Morphology", in Shopen, Timothy (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 162–164
  7. ^ Nadarajan, S. (2006). "A Crosslinguistic study of Reduplication". The Arizona Working Papers in Second Language Acquisition and Teaching. 13: 39–53.
  8. ^ Xu, D. (2012). "Reduplication in languages: A case study of languages of China". Plurality and classifiers across languages in China. Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 43–66.
  9. ^ Hsu, S.-C. (2008). "The Structure Analysis and Tone Sandhi of Reduplicative Adjectives in Taiwanese". Journal of Humanities and Social Sciences of NHCUE. 1 (1): 27–48.
  10. ^ a b Rubino, C. (2005). Reduplication: Form, function and distribution. In B. Hurch (Ed.). Studies on Reduplication (pp. 11–29). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.
  11. ^ Reid, L. A. (2009). "On the diachronic development of C1V1 reduplication in some Austronesian languages". Morphology. 19 (2): 239. doi:10.1007/s11525-009-9142-9. hdl:10125/33040. S2CID 40795368.
  12. ^ Palancar, Enrique L. & Léonard, Jean-Léo. (2014). Tone and inflection: An introduction. In Enrique L. Palancar & Jean-Léo Léonard (Eds.), Tone and Inflection: New facts under new perspectives. HAL 01099327
  13. ^ Feist, Timothy & Enrique L. Palancar. (2015). Oto-Manguean Inflectional Class Database: Tlatepuzco Chinantec. University of Surrey. doi:10.15126/SMG.28/1.01
  14. ^ Hyman, L. M. (2016). "Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?". In Palancar, E. L.; Léonard, J. L. (eds.). Tone and Inflection: New Facts and New Perspectives. Berlin, Germany: Walter de Gruyter. pp. 15–39.
  15. ^ Lyons, C. (1986). The Syntax of English Genitive Constructions. Journal of Linguistics, 22(1), 123–143.
  16. ^ Lowe, J.J. Nat Lang Linguist Theory (2016) 34: 157. doi:10.1007/s11049-015-9300-1
  17. ^ Dahl, ?sten; Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Maria (2001). The Circum-Baltic Languages: Grammar and typology. Vol. 2: Grammar and Typology. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins. p. 672.
  18. ^ Hewson, John; Bubeník, Vít (2006). From case to adposition : the development of configurational syntax in Indo-European languages. Amsterdam studies in the theory and history of linguistic science, Volume 4. Amsterdam: Benjamins. p. 206.
  19. ^ a b Ryding, Karin C. (2005). A Reference Grammar of Modern Standard Arabic.
  20. ^ a b c d e f King, Alan R. The Basque Language: A Practical Introduction. University of Nevada Press. Reno, Nevada
  21. ^ Manandise, Esméralda. "Evidence from Basque for a New Theory of Grammar", doctoral dissertation in Outstanding Dissertations in Linguistics: A Garland Series, Jorge Hankamer, general ed. Garland Publishing, Inc. New York & London.
  22. ^ Manandise, Esméralda. "Evidence from Basque for a New Theory of Grammar", doctoral dissertation in Outstanding Dissertations in Linguistics: A Garland Series, Jorge Hankamer, general ed. Garland Publishing, Inc. New York & London.
  23. ^ Norman, Jerry. (1988). Chinese (p. 98). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  24. ^ Chen, M. Y. (2000). Tone Sandhi: Patterns across Chinese dialects. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  25. ^ Lai, W.-Y. (2010). "The Source of Hakka Personal Pronoun and Genitive with the Viewpoint of Diminutive". Journal of Taiwanese Languages and Literature. 5 (1): 53–80.
  26. ^ Sun, H.-K. (1996). "Case markers of personal pronouns in Tibeto-Burman languages". Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area. 19 (2): 1–15. doi:10.32655/LTBA.19.2.01.

References

edit

Further reading

edit
  • Bauer, Laurie (2003). Introducing linguistic morphology (2nd ed.). Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press. ISBN 0-87840-343-4.
  • Haspelmath, Martin (2002). Understanding morphology. London: Arnold, Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-340-76025-7. (hb); (pbk).
  • Katamba, Francis (1993). Morphology. Modern linguistics series. New York: St. Martin's Press. ISBN 0-312-10101-5. (hb); (pbk).
  • Matthews, Peter (1991). Morphology (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-41043-6. (hb); (pbk).
  • Nichols, Johanna (1986). "Head-marking and dependent-marking grammar". Language. 62 (1): 56–119. doi:10.1353/lan.1986.0014. S2CID 144574879.
  • De Reuse, Willem J. (1996). A practical grammar of the San Carlos Apache language. LINCOM Studies in Native American Linguistics 51. LINCOM. ISBN 3-89586-861-2.
  • Spencer, Andrew; Zwicky, Arnold M., eds. (1998). The handbook of morphology. Blackwell handbooks in linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell. ISBN 0-631-18544-5.
  • Stump, Gregory T. (2001). Inflectional morphology: A theory of paradigm structure. Cambridge studies in linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-78047-0.
  • Van Valin, Robert D. Jr. (2001). An introduction to syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-63566-7. (pbk); (hb).
edit

SIL articles

edit

Lexicon of Linguistics articles

edit
维生素b族适合什么人吃 什么是禅定 ena是什么检查项目 肝多发小囊肿什么意思 高锰酸钾是什么
心脏疼挂什么科 孕妇喝什么水好 农历六月初七是什么星座 诸什么意思 湿气太重吃什么药最好
大连是什么海 叶黄素是什么东西 天秤女喜欢什么样的男生 没胃口吃什么 5月4号是什么星座
葡萄柚是什么水果 做梦梦到老婆出轨是什么意思 什么的花灯 pbc是什么 师长相当于地方什么级别
什么充电宝可以带上飞机hcv8jop4ns5r.cn 公园里有什么有什么还有什么hcv9jop5ns2r.cn 什么东西最贵hcv7jop9ns9r.cn 龙的本命佛是什么佛hcv8jop4ns8r.cn 阑尾炎不能吃什么hcv8jop5ns5r.cn
经常喝蜂蜜水有什么好处和坏处hcv7jop9ns6r.cn 小囡是什么意思hcv8jop1ns6r.cn 1981年五行属什么hcv9jop7ns3r.cn 相敬如宾是什么意思luyiluode.com 杏仁是什么hcv8jop1ns4r.cn
父母是o型血孩子是什么血型hcv9jop8ns0r.cn 葡萄糖酸钙锌口服溶液什么时候喝hcv8jop3ns9r.cn 灵魂伴侣是指什么意思wzqsfys.com 甲状腺癌有什么症状helloaicloud.com 冒失是什么意思hcv7jop9ns4r.cn
单于是什么意思hcv9jop4ns9r.cn 肩周炎是什么引起的hcv7jop7ns2r.cn 血清铁蛋白高是什么原因hcv8jop7ns4r.cn 金刚芭比什么意思hcv8jop2ns5r.cn 什么是过敏性咳嗽hcv9jop2ns6r.cn
百度